Technical Overview
Consider that many of the things
that you "know" come from experiences that you have
never had. That's one of the striking qualities of language; we
can share the experiences of others separated from us by space and
time. And though we lack first-hand knowledge, it doesn't prevent
us speaking with authority about, say, the depth of the ocean,
even though we have never seen the bottom.
In the same way, Brainhat
can function with no real knowledge of the world, given a
sufficient foundation of brokered facts to build upon.
Brainhat's vocabulary contains simple concepts.
like a ball, or the color red. These concepts are connected
hierarchically to others--e.g. balls are toys, and red is a color.
Links between the elements define the taxonomy's structure.
Everything is the child of something else, and some are the child
or parent of many.
define woman-1
label woman
child-of human-1
person first
related man-1
define human-1
label human
label person
child-of mammal-1
wants mood-1
define mammal-1
label mammal
label creature
child-of animal-1
define animal-1
label animal
child-of things
These simple
concepts can be combined to form arbitrarily complex
relationships. Within brainhat, these phrase structures are called
Complex Concepts (CC)--ideas made from other ideas.
CCs can represent elementary assertions, e.g. "the ball is
red." They can be propositions, such as "if the golden
sun is shining then beautiful people are happy." They can
also be statements of cause-and-effect--"mario is happy
because he saw the princess." CCs can even represent
questions.
Brainhat casts these Complex Concepts into inverted trees. The
constituent concepts hang from their "roots", like
mobiles of ideas. The more abstract parts of the idea (e.g.
cause-and-effect) live near the top. The actors and their
attributes (golden sun, beautiful people) live near the bottom.
The links between them define their relationships.
o Root
/ \
/ \
CAUSE / \ EFFECT
/ \
Root mario
/ | \ \
SUBJECT / | \ OBJECT \ ATTRIBUTE
/ | \ \
/ | princess happy
mario | VERB
|
saw
At runtime, CCs (e.g.
"the ball is red") are assembled, destroyed, evaluated,
compared and archived. Many live short lives as tendered (though
incorrect) interpretations of something the user may have said.
Others are inference results, generated from within the program. A few
CCs survive to become part of the context of the conversation in
progress, and to be added to the pool of things "known."
Brainhat's ability to
understand, learn, answer questions and infer are
simply the products of creation and manipulation of CCs
as part of a transformational grammar. Parsing
and pattern matching rules (context free grammar) tell brainhat how to cast
particular fragments of speech into CCs, or how to recognize a
stored idea within a CC. Processing routines manipulate the CCs to
change their meaning, or combine them to make new. Brainhat
navigates through ambiguity in language by evaluating each CC
against itself (vertically), to see whether it makes sense alone,
and against a context buffer (horizontally), to see how it fairs
against ideas that came before.
Some examples will show Brainhat in action: In this first
segment, brainhat learns about a couple of objects, and
answers some questions. Each sentence input is echoed to verify
its meaning.
>> the red ball is round
the ball is round
>> the blue ball is oval
the ball is oval
>> what shape is the red toy ?
ball is round is red
>> what color is the oval toy ?
ball is oval is blue
This next segment shows Brainhat exercising a chain of
reasoning, and explaining the outcome.
>> if thing1 is near thing2 then thing2 is near thing1
if thing1 is near thing2 then thing2 is near thing1.
>> if a man sees the princess then he is happy
if a man sees the princess then he is glad.
>> if a man is near the princess then a man can see the
princess
if a man is near the princess then he sees the princess
>> luigi is near the princess
luigi is near the princess. luigi sees the princess. he is glad.
>> why?
he is glad because luigi sees the princess.
>> why?
luigi sees the princess because luigi is near the princess.
Because Brainhat organizes concepts hierarchically, it can
apply more general cases to specific events. For instance:
>> if a person is near a thing then the person can
see the thing
if a somebody is near a something then a somebody sees a something
>> mario is near a ball
mario is near a ball. mario sees a ball
>> does mario see a toy?
yes. mario sees a ball.
>> why?
mario sees a ball because mario is near a ball
>> does he see a block
maybe. I do not know.
We can uncouple what Brainhat is told from what it believes.
In this next run, we have set a "mistrust" option to
make Brainhat more critical of its input:
>> if i say something is red then something is blue
if You say something is red then something is blue.
>> the ball is red
the ball is red. the ball is blue.
>> why?
the ball is blue because You say the ball is red.
>> do i say that the ball is red?
yes. You say the ball is red.
>> do i say that the ball is blue?
maybe. I do not know.
Underneath, parsing and processing is directed by grammar
patterns and match-processing routines.
/* Where is x?
*/
define sent-where
label question
rule where $c`tobe`0! $r`csubobj`1
map VERB,SUBJECT
mproc SPEAK
mproc CHOOSEONE
mproc PULLWHERES
mproc TOBECOMPACT
mproc PUSHTENSE
mproc REQUIREWHERES
For example, the lines
above tell brainhat how to parse and evaluate a question of
the form "where <tobe> <something>" (such as
"where are the happy people ?"). The "rule"
line gives the basic format. Sub-rules expand the
"something" portion ($r`csubobj`) and
"to-be" ($c`tobe`) portion of the question. A
"map" directive tells how the components should be
assembled into a CC. Finally, modular post-processing routines (mproc
statements) reformat the resulting CCs. Each applies some simple
processing, typically modifying the shape of the CCs that are
passed-in, and handing them off to the routine that appears above
it in the list.
New rules extend brainhat's ability to understand. As an
example, modifying the program to recognize the "where"
question in a different format is a matter of adding a second
syntax rule to the definition above, like so:
$r`csubobj`1! $c`tobe`0! where
This new pattern would match a question like "mario was where
?"
Brainhat also uses pattern matching to identify structures
within CCs. Syntactically, CC grammar patterns look very much
like input grammar patterns.
$c`color-1`0!$c`toy-1`1
The CC pattern match rule above, for example, matches any of
"red ball", "blue ball", "red
block", "pink toy", and others. The common elements
are that {red,blue,pink} and {ball,block,toy} are all
"children" of colors and toys, respectively.
This introduction was intented simply to introduce the elements of
brainhat programming. The sections that follow give more in
depth (though certainly not exhaustive) overviews of Brainhat
input grammar and vocabulary programming.
Vocabulary
Brainhat
learns about the relationships between basic concepts at start-up.
The notions that balls are toys, that pink is a shade of red, for
example, are things that you tell brainhat in advance.
Everything else (e.g. the ball is in the river) are learned as brainhat
executes.
Elements of the vocabulary are kept in a data
directory in the non-database version, or in a database
in the SQL version.
They may be created with the Brainhat development GUI or
with a file editor.
Each concept definition starts with a DEFINE
statement. A definition continues until brainhat reaches
another DEFINE or end-of-file. Within a definition are a
number of tags that identify a concepts relationships to others
around it. Concepts can be defined in any order.
However, all references should satisfied; if you refer to another
concept from within a definition, it should exist.
define block-1
label block
child-of toy-1
wants color-1
wants size-1
The sample above describes a
block. The definition has a unique name, block-1. It also
has a label, block by which you may refer to a
"block" in conversation with brainhat. Multiple
definitions may have the label block (a block can also be a
technique in American football, for example), however the
definition names should be unique. A concept can have multiple
labels, and so be known by multiple names. Each label would appear
on a line by itself.
The child-of tag identifies block1 as a more specific
example of a toy-1. Concepts can be children of any number
of other concepts (or none). Care should be taken not to create
cycles: no concept should be its own parent.
A wants tag identifies a preference for certain other
concepts that might be used in combination with it. By saying that
block-1 "wants" color, for instance, we are
specifying that if brainhat sees a block discussed in
combination with a reference to a color, we should bias our
thinking towards the toy, in lieu of a football technique.
o block-1
/|\
/ | \
CHILD / | \ WANTS
/ WANTS \
/ | \
toy-1 o | \
| o size-1
o
color-1
In some cases, we want to
identify a concept's uniqueness with respect to some parent.
Colors red and blue, for example, are unique with respect to
color. In conversation, I might refer first to a "red
ball," and then to a "blue ball." Because of your
experience with the uniqueness of color, you (as a person) will
automatically assume that I am talking about two different balls. Brainhat
makes the leap by looking at the balls' attributes, and noting
their orthogonality.
define blue-1
label blue
child-of color-1
orthogonal color-1
define red-1
label red
child-of color-1
orthogonal color-1
define pink-1
label pink
child-of red-1
orthogonal color-1
Brainhat
makes special consideration for concepts that are both orthogonal
and have a parent/child relationship. Pink will not be orthogonal
to red, but both will be orthogonal to blue.
The ultimate parent(s) of each concept determines what part of
speech it can play. Nouns must be children of things;
adjectives are children of adjective; verbs are children
of action (or tobe); prepositions are children of preposition;
articles are children of article-1,
and so on. The lineage of a ball, for example, may be ball->toy-1->things,
which makes it a candidate to fill a noun slot.
Actions (verbs) require some special handling. Brainhat
needs the freedom to handle various verb tenses. Accordingly, verb
tenses should be organized as children of the infinitive. Special
tags define the tense, number and person of
each verb. From these, brainhat can choose an appropriate tense,
number and person when speaking.
define tosee-1
label to see
child-of sense-1
define see-1
label see
child-of tosee-1
number plural
tense present
person third
define sees-1
label sees
tense present
person third
number singular
child-of tosee-1
define saw-1
label saw
child-of tosee-1
number singular
tense past
person third
The definitions above create
the infinitive form to see, and a couple of subordinate
forms. As a minimum, the infinitive and the third person singular
present form of the verb should be defined.
Input Processing
Brainhat
(as it exists today) attempts to match user input against a set of
grammar patterns, one at a time, until it finds a fit. (See the file
data/input-patterns). The "fit" is a
parts-of-speech match; it does not pre-suppose the meaning of the
matched text. Rather, many permutations may be generated, with
many different meanings. "Boy saw bat," for instance,
might generate CCs that represent "bat" as a winged
mammal, and as an wooden baseball mallet. "Saw" could
mean "viewed," or it could mean "cut in half."
As a simplification, a rule that matches "boy saw bat"
might look like this:
define xxx
label sentence
rule $c`things`0! $c`actions`1! $c`things`2
map SUBJECT,VERB,OBJECT
corresponding to "boy", "saw" and
"bat" appear in the corresponding locations. The $c`parent`x
construct says that brainhat should attempt to match a word
of type parent, and assign it to the xth position.
The "!" character indicates the termination of a
pattern component. It may or may not be needed, depending on the
character that follows.
This pattern is pretty inflexible; all parts must be present and
in the prescribed order. The good news is that the pattern can
match a wide variety of input; the sentence "ball hit
wall" could fit the pattern as well.
When an input pattern matches, many complex concepts (CCs)
are created. Each is a permutation representing a possible
interpretation of the input. The map directive describes
what the resulting CCs should look like. There will always
be a root node. From that, components hang down, one level
deep.
o Root
/|\
VERB/ | \SUBJECT
/ | \
hit o | o ball
|
OBJECT|
o wall
The map directive in
our example will create CCs like the one pictured above. In some
cases, one of the components may be specifically nominated as the
"Root." As an example, the pattern below would match
gorilla-like declarations such as "girl happy" or
"ball red."
define xxx2
label sentence
rule $c`things`0! $c`attribute-1`1
map ROOT,ATTRIBUTE
The map directive will
generate forms that Brainhat will interpret as "girl
is happy" or "ball is red" by attaching the attributes
to their subjects. The subject will assume the "Root"
position. The resulting CC would look like this:
o girl
\
\ ATTRIBUTE
\
o happy
Of course, most sentences
aren't as simple as the ones in these examples. A mildly
complicated idea may parse into CCs many levels deep. And the
sentence structure may vary widely. Accordingly, CCs are typically
constructed from other CCs. Matching decends and rises, striving
to build from the bottom up. Expanding a previous example a
little, we might match more complicated utterances such as
"the boy saw the bat," or "the boy saw mary"
using the patterns below:
define xxx3
label sentence
rule $r`subobj`0! $c`actions`1! $r`subobj`2
map SUBJECT,VERB,OBJECT
define zzz
label subobj
rule [$c`article`0! ]$c`things`1
map ATTRIBUTE,ROOT
Rules can invoke other rules:
the r`subobj'x construct instructs Brainhat
to attempt sub-rules of the type subobj and assign matches
to the SUBJECT position. By virtue of delegation the
construction of individual components (subject, object, etc.) to
other rules we can construct multi-level CCs.
o Root
/|\
VERB/ | \SUBJECT
/ | \
saw o | o boy
| \
OBJECT| \ ATTRIBUTE
o mary \
o the
Rule components that appear
in "[]"'s are optional. They are mapped if they appear
in the input stream, and ignored otherwise.
There may be multiple rules sharing a common label. These
will be tried one after another whenever $r`label`
is invoked. The first match wins. Accordingly, order matters: the
current version of Brainhat loads the rules into memory
such that the most complicated (least likely to match) form should
appear first, followed by the simplest form, and then by increasingly
more difficult forms.
Upon making a successful match, Brainhat skewers the
permutation candidates (CCs) together and passes them to post
processing routines. These routines may change the shape of the
CCs, eliminate a few, or use them for speech or to direct further
processing.
/* Where is x? */
define sent-where
label question
rule where $c`tobe`0! $r`csubobj`1
map VERB,SUBJECT
mproc SPEAK
mproc CHOOSEONE
mproc PULLWHERES
mproc TOBECOMPACT
mproc PUSHTENSE
mproc REQUIREWHERES
Post processing
routine selection starts at the bottom and proceeds upwards. In
this example, the routines are working to answer a question about
location of something. A CCs represents the question at hand.
Assume that a previous sentence told Brainhat that
"the boy is in the water." Before any post-processing,
the question "where is the boy?" might look like this:
o Root
/ \
SUBJECT / \ VERB
/ \
boy o o is
/ \ |
OBJPREP/ \PREP | TENSE
/ \ |
water o in o o present
Briefly, REQUIREWHERES
tacks a REQUIRES tag onto each of the permutation CCs. The
tag indicates that a prepositional phrase is a must-have for
answering the question. PUSHTENSE grabs the tense of the
verb and applies it to the requirement, making it further
restrictive:
Root o
/|\
SUBJECT / | \ VERB
/ | \
/ | o is
/ | |
o boy | |
| | |
ATTRIBUTE | |
| | |
Root o | |TENSE
/ \ | |
OBJPREP PREP | o present
/ \ |
o o | REQUIRES
water in |
|
|
| TENSE
Root o-------o present
OBJPREP/ \PREPOSITION
/ \
thing o o preposition
Routine TOBECOMPACT
changes the shape of the CC by removing the verb and placing the
subject in the role of "Root." PULLWHERES makes
multiple copies of the CC. Each is the same as the original except
that all but one prepositional phrase remains per copy. (In
our example there is only one prepositional phrase anyway:
"in the water.")
boy o
/|
ATTRIBUTE / |
/ |
/ |
/ |
Root o |
/ \ |
OBJPREP PREP |
/ \ |
o o | REQUIRES
water in |
|
|
| TENSE
Root o-------o present
/ \
OBJPREP/ \PREPOSITION
/ \
thing o o preposition
CHOOSEONE
selects the best result, and SPEAK voices it.
Post processing routines are many in number and function.
Vocabulary Programming
The bootstrap vocabulary for Brainhat is a collection of simple
concepts, like “ball” or “red.” These ideas are connected hierarchically
to others--e.g. balls are toys, and red is a color. Links between
the elements define the hierarchy's structure. Everything is the
child of something else, and some are the child or parent of many.
define woman-1
label woman
child human-1
person first
related man-1
define human-1
label human
label person
child mammal-1
wants mood-1
define mammal-1
label mammal
label creature
child animal-1
define animal-1
label animal
child things
Brainhat comes with a vocabulary that consists of words that
are required or sometimes used in testing. A new application may
require additional vocabulary. Let's take a few paragraphs to look
at some of the conventions.
Concepts
Concept definitions can appear in any order. One definition
ends and another begins whenever Brainhat encounters a define
statement. The name given to the definition must be unique. By
convention, one cardinally orders different uses of the same word,
e.g. ball-1, ball-2, etc., where the first might
describe a toy, the second a grand social event.
define ball-1
label ball
child toy-1
wants color-1
wants size-1
typically round-1
The sample above shows some of the basic elements of a concept
definition. Statements subsequent to define can appear
in any order. Tab characters position the columns. Most other
characters count; when defining concepts, take care to avoid trailing
blanks. Let's look at the components of a concept definition:
“label”
The label in a concept definition describes the names by which
the concept will be known. You can have as many labels as you like;
they will all act as synonyms for one another. Labels can contain
multiple words, separated by spaces.
define lollipop-1
label lollipop
label lolly
label sucker
child candy-1
“child”
The child tag describes the concept's place in the grand
scheme of things. A “poodle”, for instance, might be a child of
“dog.” In some cases, a concept will have multiple parents. Consider
a tomato: techinically, its a fruit, but most people consider it
a vegetable.
define tomato-1
label tomato
label love apple
label tomatoe
child fruit-1
child vegetable-1
The wants tag provides a little hint to Brainhat about
the usage of the concept. The definition of a ball, for example,
is more complete if you know the color and shape. A “ticket” definition
might want a cop, or might want a ballet, depending
on the kind of ticket we mean. Be careful here though; we are not
talking about what an animate object being defined might want.
Rather we are talking about other concepts that might appear in
conjunction with with the one being defined.
“orthogonal”
We can also provide hints by telling Brainhat when a concept
is exclusive of others in use. Rooms in a house are typically distinct,
for example. We might tell Brainhat that they orthogonal. That
way, when we talk about the lamp in the bedroom, Brainhat will know
that it is different than the lamp in the kitchen. You may choose
any concept as the basis for othogonality. Typically, though, one
would choose a basis that has a parent relationship.
define kitchen-1
label kitchen
child room-1
orthogonal room-1
define bedroom-1
label bedroom
child room-1
orthogonal room-1
“number”
The number tag is commonly used with verbs and nouns.
Possible values include singular and plural.
“tense”
The tense tag is primarily for use with verbs and auxiliary verbs
("enablers") (can, will, does, did). Possible
values include past, present, future imperfect,
and so-on).
“person”
Used primarily with verbs and auxiliary
verbs, the person tag can take
the values first, second
and third.
Nouns
All concepts that can trace their lineage back to “things”
can be treated as nouns. The tags we discussed above all apply
to noun concept definition. There are a few other guidelines as
well. Particularly, it helps in output generation if Brainhat knows
whether a noun is the singular or plural form of a word. And there's
also a matter of how the singular and plural forms are related in
the hierarchy.
define berth-1
label berth
label sleeping berth
label sleeping station
child place-1
child berths-1
number singular
define berths-1
label berths
label sleeping berths
label sleeping stations
child place-1
number plural
The
two concepts above are the singular and plural forms of
"berth".
They are linked together so that the singular,
"berth"
is
a child of the plural, "berths".
Both are children of "place-1" as well.
This way, we can talk about a berth as location independent
of the plural form, or we can talk about a berth as a special case
of berths. If there were a concept
"places-1"
we might link "berths-1" to that as well.
Verbs
All verbs must be able to trace their lineage back to action.
This means that, by following child links, you should
be able to find action as an ancestor. Furthermore, verbs
should be organized so that the infinitive form is a parent to
all of subordinate forms, as in this set of definitions:
define tosee-1
label to see
child sense-1
define see-1
label see
child tosee-1
number plural
tense present
person third
define sees-1
label sees
tense present
person third
number singular
child tosee-1
define saw-1
label saw
child tosee-1
number singular
tense past
person third
There is no requirement that any of the subordinate forms be
present. However, at a minimum, it is good idea that you define
the infinitive and the third person-present-singular forms. Notice
how the forms are linked together; the subordinate forms are children
of the infinitive.
Prepositions
All
prepositions must be able to trace their lineage back
to prepositions.
This means that, by following child-of
links, you should be able to find
preposition
as an ancestor.
Attributes
All attributes
must be able to trace their lineage back to attribute-1.
This means that, by following child links, you should
be able to find attribute-1 as an ancestor.
Articles
All articles
must be able to trace their lineage back to article.
This means that, by following child links, you should
be able to find article as an ancestor.
Adverbs
Adverbs have
adverb-1 as their ultimate parent.